Digital Culture :: Cognitive Context 
Key topics: brain, smartphone addiction, no-mobile-phobia, habit formation, exploitation of dopaminergic system by "social media"

Main focus of our discussions will be put on analysis of impact of digital media on psychology of children and adolescents.

At the beginning of the semester students will divide into small groups and each group will be asked to choose, read, understand and reproduce one experiment known from scientific literature.
Session 6 :: Studies to understand, explain or replicate
  • Repetitio: Cognitive sciences, cognitivism, cognitive blindness and the unicycle clown, cognitive principles and biases
  • Minilectio: Confirmation bias
  • Group work: "Digitizing literacy"
  • Initiatio to experiments of interest
  • Evaluatio
How to sign in for a specific course work
You sign in for a specific course work by bookmarking it in the Kastalia system.

1) enter the knot related to the specific experiment by clicking on its title below

2) log in if You are not logged in already

3) in section "Actions" click on "Bookmark"
Did You see a unicycling clown ?
We investigated the effects of divided attention during walking. Individuals were classified based on whether they were walking while talking on a cell phone, listening to an MP3 player, walking without any electronics or walking in a pair. In the first study, we found that cell phone users walked more slowly, changed directions more frequently, and were less likely to acknowledge other people than individuals in the other conditions. In the second study, we found that cell phone users were less likely to notice an unusual activity along their walking route (a unicycling clown). Cell phone usage may cause inattentional blindness even during a simple activity that should require few cognitive resources.
Confirmation bias
Wiki definition ::

Confirmation bias is the tendency to search for, interpret, favor, and recall information in a way that confirms one's preexisting beliefs or hypotheses.

Some psychologists restrict the term 
confirmation bias to selective collection of evidence that supports what one already believes while ignoring or rejecting evidence that supports a different conclusion. Others apply the term more broadly to the tendency to preserve one's existing beliefs when searching for evidence, interpreting it, or recalling it from memory.



Can You please present some cases of confirmation bias from everyday life ?
What the Thinker thinks the Prover proves
Digitizing Literacy :: Reflections on haptics of writing
Writing is a complex cognitive process relying on intricate perceptual-sensorimotor combinations. The process and skill of writing is studied on several levels and in many disciplines, from neurophysiological research on the shaping of each letter to studies on stylistic and compositional features of authors and poets. In studies of writing and literacy overall, the role of the physically tangible writing device (pen on paper; computer mouse and keyboard; digital stylus pen and writing tablet; etc.) is rarely addressed. By and large, the (relatively young) field of writing research is dominated by cognitive approaches predominantly focusing on the visual component of the writing process, hence maintaining a separation between (visual) perception and motor action (e.g., haptics1). However, recent theoretical currents in psychology, phenomenology & philosophy of mind, and neuroscience – commonly referred to as “embodied cognition” – indicate that perception and motor action are closely connected and, indeed, reciprocally dependent. 
Problematic smartphone use and relations with negative affect, fear of missing out, and fear of negative and positive evaluation
ABSTRACT
For many individuals, excessive smartphone use interferes with everyday life. In the present study, we recruited a non-clinical sample of 296 participants for a cross-sectional survey of problematic smartphone use, social and non-social smartphone use, and psychopathology-related constructs including negative affect, fear of negative and positive evaluation, and fear of missing out (FoMO). Results demonstrated that FoMO was most strongly related to both problematic smartphone use and social smartphone use relative to negative affect and fears of negative and positive evaluation, and these relations held when controlling for age and gender. Furthermore, FoMO (cross-sectionally) mediated relations between both fear of negative and positive evaluation with both problematic and social smartphone use. Theoretical implications are considered with regard to developing problematic smartphone use.
"Brain Drain: The Mere Presence of One’s Own Smartphone Reduces Available Cognitive Capacity"
ABSTRACT Our smartphones enable—and encourage—constant connection to information, entertainment, and each other. They put the world at our fingertips, and rarely leave our sides. Although these devices have immense potential to improve welfare, their persistent presence may come at a cognitive cost. In this research, we test the “brain drain” hypothesis that the mere presence of one’s own smartphone may occupy limited-capacity cognitive resources, thereby leaving fewer resources available for other tasks and undercutting cognitive performance. Results from two experiments indicate that even when people are successful at maintaining sustained attention—as when avoiding the temptation to check their phones—the mere presence of these devices reduces available cognitive capacity. Moreover, these cognitive costs are highest for those highest in smartphone dependence. We conclude by discussing the practical implications of this smartphone-induced brain drain for consumer decision-making and consumer welfare.
Quantitative :: Digital devices in the public space
The objective is to estimate the impact of digital media on human society in June 2019 (AE49).
What You'll need is:

1) Few print-outs of the Media-to-Human protocol (M2H-protocol) , c.f. below
2) Systematic approach

Comfortably Numb Desensitizing Effects of Violent Media on Helping Others
ABSTRACT

Two studies tested the hypothesis that exposure to violent media reduces aid offered to people in pain. In Study 1, participants played a violent or nonviolent video game for 20 min. After game play, while completing a lengthy questionnaire, they heard a loud fight, in which one person was injured, outside the lab. Participants who played violent games took longer to help the injured victim, rated the fight as less serious, and were less likely to ‘‘hear’’ the fight in comparison to participants who played nonviolent games. In Study 2, violent- and nonviolentmovie attendees witnessed a young woman with an injured ankle struggle to pick up her crutches outside the theater either before or after the movie. Participants who had just watched a violent movie took longer to help than participants in the other three conditions. The findings from both studies suggest that violent media make people numb to the pain and suffering of others.
Increases in Depressive Symptoms, Suicide-Related Outcomes, and Suicide Rates Among U.S. Adolescents After 2010 and Links to Increased New Media Screen Time
Abstract
In two nationally representative surveys of U.S. adolescents in grades 8 through 12 (N = 506,820) and national statistics on suicide deaths for those ages 13 to 18, adolescents’ depressive symptoms, suicide-related outcomes, and suicide rates increased between 2010 and 2015, especially among females. Adolescents who spent more time on new media (including social media and electronic devices such as smartphones) were more likely to report mental health issues, and adolescents who spent more time on nonscreen activities (in-person social interaction, sports/exercise, homework, print media, and attending religious services) were less likely. Since 2010, iGen adolescents have spent more time on new media screen activities and less time on nonscreen activities, which may account for the increases in depression and suicide. In contrast, cyclical economic factors such as unemployment and the Dow Jones Index were not linked to depressive symptoms or suicide rates when matched by year.
Exploring the dimensions of nomophobia: Development and validation of a self-reported questionnaire
abstract

Nomophobia is considered a modern age phobia introduced to our lives as a byproduct of the interaction between people and mobile information and communication technologies, especially smartphones. This study sought to contribute to the nomophobia research literature by identifying and describing the dimensions of nomophobia and developing a questionnaire to measure nomophobia. Consequently, this study adopted a two-phase, exploratory sequential mixed methods design. The first phase was a qualitative exploration of nomophobia through semi-structured interviews conducted with nine undergraduate students at a large Midwestern university in the U.S. As a result of the first phase, four dimensions of nomophobia were identified: not being able to communicate, losing connectedness, not being able to access information and giving up convenience. The qualitative findings from this initial exploration were then developed into a 20-item nomophobia questionnaire (NMP-Q). In the second phase, the NMP-Q was validated with a sample of 301 undergraduate students. Exploratory factor analysis revealed a four-factor structure for the NMP-Q, corresponding to the dimensions of nomophobia. The NMP-Q was shown to produce valid and reliable scores; and thus, can be used to assess the severity of nomophobia
Qualitative :: Gestalt Therapy and the digital world
The goal is to sharpen Your awareness concerning the interaction between humans and machines.

Task 0) Here and now, I am observing a human using a digital device ...
Task 1) Here and now, I do not percieve anyone using a digital device...
Task 2) Here and now, I see a digital device being carried around by a human being...
Task 3) ...

Output: Self-observation protocol.
Experiments
Please choose one among following experiments on presentation/realisation of which You shall participate. 
Program
Date Topic
12.4 Introduction
19.4 NO COURSE (Karfreitag)
26.4 How to read scientific articles
3.5 Google, Brain & co.
10.5 Cognitive sciences
17.5 Cognitive psychology
24.5 Abstracts
31.5 OPTIONAL COURSE (Christihimmelfahrt Brückentag)
7.6 Memetic theory
14.6 Theory of multiple intelligences
21.6 Developmental aspects
28.6 Socrates & Gestalt
5.7 Symposion
12.7 Summa Summarum
Digital Culture :: Cognitive Context :: Handouts
Stuff to read.
Digitizing Literacy :: Reflections on haptics of writing
Writing is a complex cognitive process relying on intricate perceptual-sensorimotor combinations. The process and skill of writing is studied on several levels and in many disciplines, from neurophysiological research on the shaping of each letter to studies on stylistic and compositional features of authors and poets. In studies of writing and literacy overall, the role of the physically tangible writing device (pen on paper; computer mouse and keyboard; digital stylus pen and writing tablet; etc.) is rarely addressed. By and large, the (relatively young) field of writing research is dominated by cognitive approaches predominantly focusing on the visual component of the writing process, hence maintaining a separation between (visual) perception and motor action (e.g., haptics1). However, recent theoretical currents in psychology, phenomenology & philosophy of mind, and neuroscience – commonly referred to as “embodied cognition” – indicate that perception and motor action are closely connected and, indeed, reciprocally dependent. 
Small et al. :: Meet Your iBrain
Easier-to-read introduction Small et al. (2009) 
Small et al. (2009) :: Your Brain on Google: Patterns of Cerebral Activation during Internet Searching
Objective: Previous research suggests that engaging in mentally stimulating tasks may im-

prove brain health and cognitive abilities. Using computer search engines to find information

on the Internet has become a frequent daily activity of people at any age, including middle-

aged and older adults. As a preliminary means of exploring the possible influence of Internet

experience on brain activation patterns, the authors performed functional magnetic resonance

imaging (MRI) of the brain in older persons during search engine use and explored whether

prior search engine experience was associated with the pattern of brain activation during

Internet use. Design: Cross-sectional, exploratory observational study Participants: The au-

thors studied 24 subjects (age, 55–76 years) who were neurologically normal, of whom 12 had

minimal Internet search engine experience (Net Naive group) and 12 had more extensive

experience (Net Savvy group). The mean age and level of education were similar in the two

groups. Measurements: Patterns of brain activation during functional MRI scanning were

determined while subjects performed a novel Internet search task, or a control task of reading

text on a computer screen formatted to simulate the prototypic layout of a printed book, where

the content was matched in all respects, in comparison with a nontext control task. Results:

The text reading task activated brain regions controlling language, reading, memory, and

visual abilities, including left inferior frontal, temporal, posterior cingulate, parietal, and

occipital regions, and both the magnitude and the extent of brain activation were similar in

the Net Naive and Net Savvy groups. During the Internet search task, the Net Naive group

showed an activation pattern similar to that of their text reading task, whereas the Net Savvy

group demonstrated significant increases in signal intensity in additional regions controlling

decision making, complex reasoning, and vision, including the frontal pole, anterior temporal

region, anterior and posterior cingulate, and hippocampus. Internet searching was associated

with a more than twofold increase in the extent of activation in the major regional clusters in

the Net Savvy group compared with the Net Naive group (21,782 versus 8,646 total activated

voxels). Conclusion: Although the present findings must be interpreted cautiously in light of

the exploratory design of this study, they suggest that Internet searching may engage a greater

extent of neural circuitry not activated while reading text pages but only in people with prior

computer and Internet search experience. These observations suggest that in middle-aged and

older adults, prior experience with Internet searching may alter the brain’s responsiveness in

neural circuits controlling decision making and complex reasoning.(Am J Geriatr Psychiatry

2009; 17:116 –126)


Key Words: Brain activation, functional MRI, Internet search, middle-age and older

adults, computer experience
Mathematics of a Lady Tasting Tea

In the design of experiments in statistics, the lady tasting tea is a randomized experiment devised by Ronald Fisher and reported in his book The Design of Experiments (1935).[1] The experiment is the original exposition of Fisher's notion of a null hypothesis, which is "never proved or established, but is possibly disproved, in the course of experimentation".[2][3]


The lady in question (Muriel Bristol) claimed to be able to tell whether the tea or the milk was added first to a cup. Fisher proposed to give her eight cups, four of each variety, in random order. One could then ask what the probability was for her getting the specific number of cups she identified correct, but just by chance.


Fisher's description is less than 10 pages in length and is notable for its simplicity and completeness regarding terminology, calculations and design of the experiment

Session 2 :: How to read and understand scientific papers.
Presentation by Dr. M. Mihalilkova.
Did You see a unicycling clown ?
We investigated the effects of divided attention during walking. Individuals were classified based on whether they were walking while talking on a cell phone, listening to an MP3 player, walking without any electronics or walking in a pair. In the first study, we found that cell phone users walked more slowly, changed directions more frequently, and were less likely to acknowledge other people than individuals in the other conditions. In the second study, we found that cell phone users were less likely to notice an unusual activity along their walking route (a unicycling clown). Cell phone usage may cause inattentional blindness even during a simple activity that should require few cognitive resources.
Nicolas Carr :: 2008 :: Is Google making us stupid ?

Is Google Making Us Stupid? is a 2008 article written by technologist Nicholas Carr for The Atlantic, and later expanded on in a published edition by W. W. Norton. The book investigates the cognitive effects of technological advancements that relegate certain cognitive activities — namely, knowledge-searching — to external computational devices. The book received mainstream recognition for interrogating the assumptions people make about technological change and advocating for a component of personal accountability in our relationships to devices.


Carr begins the essay by saying that his recent problems with concentrating on reading lengthy texts, including the books and articles that he used to read effortlessly, stem from spending too much time on the Internet. He suggests that constantly using the Internet might reduce one’s ability to concentrate and reflect on content. He introduces a few anecdotes taken from bloggers who write about the transformation in their reading and writing habits over time. In addition, he analyzes a 2008 study by University College London about new “types” of reading that will emerge and become predominant in the information age. He particularly refers to the work of Maryanne Wolf, a reading behavior scholar, which includes theories about the role of technology and media in learning how to write new languages. Carr argues that while speech is an innate ability that stems directly from brain structure, reading is conscious and taught. He acknowledges that this theory has a paucity of evidence so far, but refers to such works as Wolf’s Proust and the Squid, which discusses how the brain’s neurons adapt to a creature’s environmental demands to become literate in new problem areas. The Internet, in his opinion, is just another kind of environment that we will uniquely adapt to.


Carr discusses how concentration might be impaired by Internet usage. He references the historical example of Nietzsche, who used a typewriter, which was new during his time in the 1880s. Allegedly, Nietzsche’s writing style changed after the advent of the typewriter. Carr categorizes this example as demonstrative of neuroplasticity, a scientific theory that states neural circuits are contingent and in flux. He invokes the idea of sociologist Daniel Bell that technologies extend human cognition, arguing that humans unconsciously conform to the very qualities, or kinds of patterns, involved in these devices’ functions. He uses the clock as an example of a device that has both improved and regulated human perception and behavior.


Carr argues that the Internet is changing behavior at unprecedented levels because it is one of the most pervasive and life-altering technologies in human history. He suggests that the Internet engenders cognitive distractions in the form of ads and popups. These concentration-altering events are only worsened by online media as they adapt their strategies and visual forms to those of Internet platforms to seem more legitimate and trick the viewer into processing them.


Carr also posits that people’s ability to concentrate might decrease as new algorithms free us from knowledge work; that is, the process of manipulating and synthesizing abstract information into new concepts and conclusions. He compares the Internet with industrial management systems, tracing how they caused workers to complain that they felt like automata after the implementation of Taylorist management workflows. He compares this example with the modern example of Google, which places its computer engineers and designers into a systematized knowledge environment, creating robust insights and results at the expense of creativity. Additionally, Carr argues that the Internet makes its money mainly by exploiting users’ privacy or bombarding them with overstimulation, a vicious cycle where companies facilitate mindless browsing instead of rewarding sustained thinking.


Carr ends his essay by tracing the roots of the skeptic trend. He discusses events where people were wary about new technologies, including Socrates’s skepticism about the use of written language and a fifteenth-century Italian editor’s concern about the shift from manually written to printed works. All of these technologies indelibly changed human cognition, but also led to mind-opening innovations that endure today. Still, Carr concludes his argument on an ambivalent note, citing a quote by Richard Foreman that laments the erosion of educated and articulate people. Though Google and other knowledge-finding and knowledge-building technologies might speed up existing human computational processes, they might also foreclose the human potential to easily create new knowledge.

[Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Is_Google_Making_Us_Stupid%3F#Synopsis ]

Small treatise concerning the concepts of «invasivity» and «reversibility» and their relation to past, present and future techniques of neural imagery
Introduction The aim of this text is threefold: Firstly, to prove to the Teacher that the author of this article (i.e. Student) have sufficiently internalized all the facts presented during UE Neuroimagery. Secondly, Student aims to introduce the notion of «invasivity» as something which should be considered wery seriously by someone who seeks an «ideal method» of conducting his future (neuro)scientific experiments towards success. But the ultimate aim si to show that certain «philosophical schools» who point out to «invasivity-related aspects» of current neuro-scientific research are not doing so from the position of moralizing savants locked in their ivory towers, but they do so because of concrete and highly-pragmatic reasons related to purest expressions of highest scientific practice. Principal thesis of this text states that « invasivity » and « reversibility » aspects of a chosen experimental method should determine experimentator's choice at least as significantly as other aspects like spatial/temporal resolution characteristics, signal/noise ratio or economical feasibility. First part of the text is dedicated to highly invasive techniques tissue extraction and analysis by means of electron, multiphoton or confocal microscopes. Post mortem autopsy and chirurgical interventions like vivisesction or lobotomy will be mentioned when discussing this group. Common demoninator of these approaches is that their condition sine qua non of their realisation is nonreversible and fatal degradation of one vital functions of the organism under study or...death. Second part of the text is dedicated to somewhat more reversible, nonetheless still very brutal «in vivo» techniques like that of calcic imaging, optic imaging or electrode implantation. Because it is evident that such approaches can inflict severe injuries and suffering of the organisms under study, they will be labeled as «partially reversible quasi in vivo techniques». Contrary to common categorisation of these days, even techniques like PET (positron emission tomography) or X-ray imaging will be included into this middle group of partially invasive techniques. This is due to their high-energy kinship with radioactivity which can without any doubt induce mutations resulting in the disequilibrium of a living system which is commonly known as «loss of health». The loss of this precious equilibrium is the reason why we'll include all the luminescence/fluorescence marker techniques into this category as well. The third part of the text aims to bring hope. It will be fully devoted to techniques which can be considered as fully reversible: focus will be definitely on Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Electroencephalography (EEG) while other non-invasive techniques (NIRS, echography or TCD) will be excluded from the list due to lack of Student's personal experience with these techniques. The small part of this final part will be dedicated to «what if?» speculation proposing to use these pure and elegant techniques not only for imaging, but as well as a tool of healing practice. These three parts can be considered as a core of Student's homework demanding him to «highlight the advantages and limits of these techniques depending from the scientific question You'll pose». The question posed by student is this: «According to what criteriae could we possibly quantify invasivity of an experimental tool or method ? » This text will try to answer this question by introducing the term which we label hereby as «Information/Invasivity Quotient» (IIQ).We'll analyse this notion from more ethical perspective in Discussion section,while Appendix will summariz IIQ-based ranking of 4 presented methods.
Contacts
Prof. Dr. Dr. Daniel Hromada


  • daniel at udk-berlin.de

  • Room 313, Medienhaus

  • Sprechstunden 12:30 - 13:30


Tutors & SHK


  • Astrid Kraniger a.kraniger@udk-berlin.de

  • Nikoloz Kapanadze nikoloz-kapanadze@medienhaus.udk-berlin.de

Net links

  • http://bildung.digital.udk-berlin.de/prolegomena2

  • https://kastalia.medienhaus.udk-berlin.de/4161

  • Astrid will give You Your kastalia login / password next week

Credits & co.

  • You can validate the course either as Wissenschaften / Theorie fach or Studium Generale course

  • in both cases I can give only 2 credits *

  • those who opt for the Studium Generale option and do particulary well can get note "eins" auf SG-schein


* but we do not study because of credits, do we ?
Course validation
For all:


  • attendance at least 60% of seminars obligatory

  • participation in the group work


For those who want to validate this course as Studium Generale


  • if You visit more than 10 out of 12 sessions and do the group work, You're done

  • if You visit more than 7 courses, You'll have to do additional compensation work

Program
Date Topic
12.4 Introduction
19.4 NO COURSE (Karfreitag)
26.4 How to read scientific articles
3.5 Google, Brain & co.
10.5 Cognitive sciences
17.5 Cognitive psychology
24.5 Abstracts
31.5 OPTIONAL COURSE (Christihimmelfahrt Brückentag)
7.6 Memetic theory
14.6 Theory of multiple intelligences
21.6 Developmental aspects
28.6 Socrates & Gestalt
5.7 Symposion
12.7 Summa Summarum
Bibliography
Manfred Spitzer - Digitale Demenz; Smartphone Epidemy
Introductory video
Goals

  • Introduce You to cognitive sciences

  • Show You how to work with scientific literature

  • Get our digital habits under control

  • do some experiments

  • learn something from You and have fun

F.A.Q.
If You have any course-related questions, please ask them here.

(use the "Create child knot" button below to pose a new question)
Session 3 :: Google, Brain & co.
  1. Repetitio: Sessions 1 (Introduction) and 2 (How to read scientific articles)
  2. Group work 1 : Carr (2008) Is Google making us stupid ?
  3. Lectio: Brain
  4. Group work 2 : Small et al. (2009) Your Brain on Google: Patterns of Cerebral Activation during Internet Searching
  5. Anticipatio

ACHTUNG :: Today's session will be most probably the most difficult one.

Motor cortex
Image result for motor cortex
functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)
Image result for fmri
Brain

Human brain is a physical (i.e. four-dimensional) object of organic origin which consumes biochemical energy in order to process and/or store information in a non-local, highly parallel, and in certain extent also plastic, equipotent and holographic fashion robust to both endogenous and exogenous perturbations.

(Hromada (2019), Prolegomena Paedagogica, page 7)

Hexagram of cognitive sciences
Image result for hexagram of cognitive sciences
Brain atlas
Sensory cortex
Image result for sensory cortex
Groups
Group 1 - Aaron, Philip
Group 2 - Frederico, Hannes
Group 3 - Astrid, Maja, Kohei
Group 4 - Adam, Patrick
Group 5 - Ozcan, Akif, Anna-Luisa
Session 4 :: Cognitive context

  1. Repetitio

  2. Group work 1: Interpreting Small et al. (2009)

  3. Lectio: Cognition, cognitive sciences, cognitive functions, cognitive psychology

  4. Group work 2: Sentiment detection in "Meet Your iBrain" article

  5. Your evaluation of this course

  6. Experiment 0

Your evaluation of this course

  1. What do You like about this course ?

  2. What do You not like ?

  3. What are Your wishes ?

cognition (n.)

cognition (n.)



mid-15c., cognicioun, "ability to comprehend, mental act or process of knowing," from Latin cognitionem(nominative cognitio) "a getting to know, acquaintance, knowledge," noun of action from past participle stem of cognoscere "to get to know, recognize," from assimilated form of com"together" (see co-) + gnoscere "to know," from PIE root *gno- "to know." In 17c. the meaning was extended to include perception and sensation.

https://www.etymonline.com/word/cognition


Hexagram of cognitive sciences
Image result for hexagram of cognitive sciences
Cognitive functions and processes
Cognitive functions, also referred to as psychological functions, as described by Carl Jung in his book Psychological Types, are particular mental processes within a person's psyche that are present regardless of common circumstance. This was a concept that served as one of the conceptual foundations for his theory on personality type. In his book, he noted four main psychological functions: thinkingfeelingsensation, and intuition. He introduced them with having either an internally focused (introverted) or externally focused (extraverted) tendency which he called "attitudes".

But current cognitive sciences rather tend to speak about cognitive processes: these include problem solving, ...

Experiment 0
Whenever You see a person interacting with a digital device, tell Yourself something like:

Here and now I see a human being using a smartphone. Here and now the woman in front of me is touching the screen of her smartphone...


Make sure that You do Your best to try all sentences with "here and now".
Session 2 :: How to read and understand scientific papers.
Presentation by Dr. M. Mihalilkova.
Session 5 :: Cognitive psychology
    1. Repetitio

    2. Summing up Small et al.

    3. Lectio: cognitivism, mind, cognitive processes, cognitive biases, cognitive principles, embodied cognition

    4. Your evaluation of this course

    5. Work: Digitizing literacy: reflections on haptics of writing

    6. Experiment 1
Small & Vorgan :: Meet Your iBrain (2008)

  • published in a October/November 2008 issue of popularisation review Scientific American Mind

  • mentions the fMRI study (c.f. next slide), but only in the state where 3 Net Naive volunteers were recruited (c.f. page 45)

  • quite techno-pessimist terminology: "continuous partial attention", "heightened state of stress", "a sense of constant crisis", "digital fog" (p. 47), "techno-brain burn-out is threating to become an epidemic", "impair cognition, lead to depression ..." (p.48)

  • little bit of techno-optimism at the end: "technological experiences sharpen some cognitive abilities" (p. 49)

  • summing up: "all of us... will master new technologies and take advantage of their efficiencies, but we need to maintain our people skills and our humanity"

Cognitive / mental processes

    • perception
    • decision-making
    • problem-solving
    • ... what else ?
    • attention / awareness
a little awareness tests ...
Small et al. :: Your Brain on Google (2009)

  • serious scientific article published in February 2009 edition of a peer-reviewed journal Geriatric Psychiatry

  • compares statistically significantly different groups of Net Naive (N=12) and Net Savvy (N=12) seniors

  • Main result :: "The most striking finding was in the direct comparison of the Internet versus text reading tasks for the Net Naive and Net Savvy groups, which found that the Net Savvy group had more than a twofold greater spatial extent of activation than did the Net Naive group during the Internet task".

  • Positive opinion: "present results are encouraging that emerging computerized technologies designed to improve cognitive abilities and brain function may have physiologic effects and potential benefits for middle-aged and older adults" (p.125)

  • Negative opinion: "constant use of such technologies have the potential for negative brain and behavioral effects, including impaired attention and addiction"

  •  

Definition of "cognitivism" and definition of "mind"

"cognitivism" = "behaviorism" + "mind"

"An auto-organising set of structures and processes determining the characteristic behaviour of an individual." (Prolegomena Peadagogica, 2019, p.2)

cognitive biases etc.
cognitive bias is a systematic pattern of deviation from norm or rationality in judgment. Individuals create their own "subjective social reality" from their perception of the input. An individual's construction of social reality, not the objective input, may dictate their behaviour in the social world. Thus, cognitive biases may sometimes lead to perceptual distortion, inaccurate judgment, illogical interpretation, or what is broadly called irrationality.

Some cognitive biases are presumably adaptive. Cognitive biases may lead to more effective actions in a given context. Furthermore, allowing cognitive biases enable faster decisions which can be desirable when timeliness is more valuable than accuracy, as illustrated in heuristics.

Definitely check the List of cognitive biases
An example of a "cognitive principle"

Given an equal number of exposures, distributed (or spaced) practice at a skill is almost always superior to massed practice.» (Tomasello, 2009; Prolegomena, 2019, p. 102)

Experiments
Please choose one among following experiments on presentation/realisation of which You shall participate. 
Exploring the dimensions of nomophobia: Development and validation of a self-reported questionnaire
abstract

Nomophobia is considered a modern age phobia introduced to our lives as a byproduct of the interaction between people and mobile information and communication technologies, especially smartphones. This study sought to contribute to the nomophobia research literature by identifying and describing the dimensions of nomophobia and developing a questionnaire to measure nomophobia. Consequently, this study adopted a two-phase, exploratory sequential mixed methods design. The first phase was a qualitative exploration of nomophobia through semi-structured interviews conducted with nine undergraduate students at a large Midwestern university in the U.S. As a result of the first phase, four dimensions of nomophobia were identified: not being able to communicate, losing connectedness, not being able to access information and giving up convenience. The qualitative findings from this initial exploration were then developed into a 20-item nomophobia questionnaire (NMP-Q). In the second phase, the NMP-Q was validated with a sample of 301 undergraduate students. Exploratory factor analysis revealed a four-factor structure for the NMP-Q, corresponding to the dimensions of nomophobia. The NMP-Q was shown to produce valid and reliable scores; and thus, can be used to assess the severity of nomophobia
"Brain Drain: The Mere Presence of One’s Own Smartphone Reduces Available Cognitive Capacity"
ABSTRACT Our smartphones enable—and encourage—constant connection to information, entertainment, and each other. They put the world at our fingertips, and rarely leave our sides. Although these devices have immense potential to improve welfare, their persistent presence may come at a cognitive cost. In this research, we test the “brain drain” hypothesis that the mere presence of one’s own smartphone may occupy limited-capacity cognitive resources, thereby leaving fewer resources available for other tasks and undercutting cognitive performance. Results from two experiments indicate that even when people are successful at maintaining sustained attention—as when avoiding the temptation to check their phones—the mere presence of these devices reduces available cognitive capacity. Moreover, these cognitive costs are highest for those highest in smartphone dependence. We conclude by discussing the practical implications of this smartphone-induced brain drain for consumer decision-making and consumer welfare.
Comfortably Numb Desensitizing Effects of Violent Media on Helping Others
ABSTRACT

Two studies tested the hypothesis that exposure to violent media reduces aid offered to people in pain. In Study 1, participants played a violent or nonviolent video game for 20 min. After game play, while completing a lengthy questionnaire, they heard a loud fight, in which one person was injured, outside the lab. Participants who played violent games took longer to help the injured victim, rated the fight as less serious, and were less likely to ‘‘hear’’ the fight in comparison to participants who played nonviolent games. In Study 2, violent- and nonviolentmovie attendees witnessed a young woman with an injured ankle struggle to pick up her crutches outside the theater either before or after the movie. Participants who had just watched a violent movie took longer to help than participants in the other three conditions. The findings from both studies suggest that violent media make people numb to the pain and suffering of others.
Quantitative :: Digital devices in the public space
The objective is to estimate the impact of digital media on human society in June 2019 (AE49).
What You'll need is:

1) Few print-outs of the Media-to-Human protocol (M2H-protocol) , c.f. below
2) Systematic approach

Referat structure :: IMRAD
Introduction

Method

Results

Discussion
Problematic smartphone use and relations with negative affect, fear of missing out, and fear of negative and positive evaluation
ABSTRACT
For many individuals, excessive smartphone use interferes with everyday life. In the present study, we recruited a non-clinical sample of 296 participants for a cross-sectional survey of problematic smartphone use, social and non-social smartphone use, and psychopathology-related constructs including negative affect, fear of negative and positive evaluation, and fear of missing out (FoMO). Results demonstrated that FoMO was most strongly related to both problematic smartphone use and social smartphone use relative to negative affect and fears of negative and positive evaluation, and these relations held when controlling for age and gender. Furthermore, FoMO (cross-sectionally) mediated relations between both fear of negative and positive evaluation with both problematic and social smartphone use. Theoretical implications are considered with regard to developing problematic smartphone use.
Qualitative :: Gestalt Therapy and the digital world
The goal is to sharpen Your awareness concerning the interaction between humans and machines.

Task 0) Here and now, I am observing a human using a digital device ...
Task 1) Here and now, I do not percieve anyone using a digital device...
Task 2) Here and now, I see a digital device being carried around by a human being...
Task 3) ...

Output: Self-observation protocol.
Increases in Depressive Symptoms, Suicide-Related Outcomes, and Suicide Rates Among U.S. Adolescents After 2010 and Links to Increased New Media Screen Time
Abstract
In two nationally representative surveys of U.S. adolescents in grades 8 through 12 (N = 506,820) and national statistics on suicide deaths for those ages 13 to 18, adolescents’ depressive symptoms, suicide-related outcomes, and suicide rates increased between 2010 and 2015, especially among females. Adolescents who spent more time on new media (including social media and electronic devices such as smartphones) were more likely to report mental health issues, and adolescents who spent more time on nonscreen activities (in-person social interaction, sports/exercise, homework, print media, and attending religious services) were less likely. Since 2010, iGen adolescents have spent more time on new media screen activities and less time on nonscreen activities, which may account for the increases in depression and suicide. In contrast, cyclical economic factors such as unemployment and the Dow Jones Index were not linked to depressive symptoms or suicide rates when matched by year.
Experiments
Please choose one among following experiments on presentation/realisation of which You shall participate. 
Session 7 :: Memetic Theory

  1. Evaluation

  2. Experiment choice

  3. Theory of memes

Take home lesson
There's a lot of activity going on in our mindbrain and some thoughts / behaviours / needs acting in our brain have their "own" interests!
 
Freud's iceberg model of unconscious, pre-conscious and conscious levels  
P.S. and it may be the case that machines, "they themselves", have also their "own" replicatory interests
Replicators of the second kind
Blackmore (2000) defines "Meme is a replicator which replicates from brain to brain by means of imitation" . These replicators are somehow represented in the host brain as some kind of «cognitive structure» and if ever externalised by the host organism – no matter whether in form a word, song, behavioral schema or an artefact – they can get copied into other host organism endowed with the device to integrate such structures.

(Prolegomena Paedagogica, pp. 37-38)
The turning point
It is not plain "humans VERB machines/devices" anymore.

It starts to be more "machines/devices are PAST_PARTICIPLE(VERB) by humans"

VERB can be something like "use/used by; carry/carried by; create/created by"
Internalists and externalists
Internalists define memes as informatic structures located in the brain.

Externalities define memes in terms of their observable expressions (cultural artifacts, behaviors etc.)

What is common to both camps is that they both attribute memes their own "urge to reproduce themselves", which is independent (and sometimes even contradictory) to needs of the "host" organism.


examples: language, rituals, songs, taking selfies, internet memes, chain letters, fashions and styles, WC prose, earworms, what else ?
 
Replicator dissonance
"In order to reproduce, replicators consume energy." (Prolegomena, pp.6)

Given that the amount of available energy is always finite within an individual host, different replicators can get into conflict within individual hosts.

Take, as an example conflict between the memetic complex called "catholic doctrine" and gene-determined organic nature of an average Jesuit missionary.

Or a fanatic which willing to kill himself (and/or other people) just for the purpose of getting his lethal meme into media.

Or a conflict between conservative / liberal, left / right, carnivore / vegetarian memplexes etc.


Evaluation
In following 10-15 minutes please describe:


  1. POSITIVES :: What do You like about this course ? Did You learn something useful ?

  2. NEGATIVES :: What do You NOT like about this course ? Do You find something confusing / chaotic / difficult to understand ?

  3. CONSTRUCTIVES :: What are Your wishes for the rest of the semester ? Is there some specific topic related to cognition and/or digital technologies You would like to discuss ?

Experiment choice
Please go to https://kastalia.medienhaus.udk-berlin.de/4406/, click on the title of experiment which interests You the most and assign it by bookmarking it (Actions->bookmark).
The meme
The term meme was coined in Richard Dawkins' 1976 book The Selfish Gene, but Dawkins later distanced himself from the resulting field of study. Analogous to a gene, the meme was conceived as a "unit of culture" (an idea, belief, pattern of behaviour, etc.) which is "hosted" in the minds of one or more individuals, and which can reproduce itself in the sense of jumping from the mind of one person to the mind of another. Thus what would otherwise be regarded as one individual influencing another to adopt a belief is seen as an idea-replicator reproducing itself in a new host.

(Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Memetics )
Kinds of replicators

  1. genes

  2. memes

  3. ... ???

  4. ... ???

Session 7 :: Theory of multiple intelligences

  1. Repetitio

  2. Theory of multiple intelligences

  3. Experiments

  4. After smartphone: towards a new digital education artefact 

After smartphone: towards a new digital education artefact

Abstract. Non-moderate smartphone usage may induce diverse pathological states and behaviors which may potentially result in an array of syndromes and illnesses. Digital devices built for education rather than consumption and entertainment should not neglect intricacies of human physiology, ergonomy and cognition. For this reason, we present first four properties of an idealized ”digital primer” artefact which could maximize the human and cultural potential of a normal elementary school pupil by means of holistic, semi-supervised interaction. Properties addressed and defined in this article are: ”speech-based”, ”narrative”, ”circa-temporal” and ”habit-disrupting”.


 


Key-words. digital primer, speech-based, circa-temporal, narrative, habit-disrupting, smartphone epidemic

Intelligence

  • Autonomous decision making

  • Ability to react to environment to improve own state

  • Ability to understand

  • About reflecting ("the more one reflects, the more intelligent one is")

  • Understanding abstract concepts ("ability to imagine which is not there/tangible")

  • Withstand impulsive behaviour

Session 8 :: Developmental aspects

  1. Repetitio (Theory of multiple intelligences)

  2. Some administrative stuff (we meet in Medienhaus garden next week, 12th July course etc.)

  3. Your experiments

  4. Developmental aspects

  5. The nightmare videos of children's Youtube

  6. After smartphone: towards a new digital education artefact 

The nightmare videos of children's Youtube
Developmental aspects
Die Lüge der digitalen Bildung - Gerald Lembke, Ingo Leipner

Core thesis:  "Eine Kindheit ohne Computer ist der beste Start ins digitale Zeitalter."
American Pediatric Association Recommendations

For children younger than 18 months, use of screen media other than video-chatting should be discouraged.


Parents of children 18 to 24 months of age who want to introduce digital media should choose high-quality programming/apps and use them together with children, because this is how toddlers learn best. Letting children use media by themselves should be avoided.


For children older than 2 years, media limits are very appropriate. Limit screen use to no more than 1 hour or less per day of high-quality programming. Co-view or co-play with your children, and find other activities for to do together that are healthy for the body and mind (e.g., reading, teaching, talking, and playing together).


All children and teens need adequate sleep (8-12 hours, depending on age), physical activity (1 hour), and time away from media. Designate media-free times together (e.g., family dinner) and media-free zones (e.g., bedrooms). Children should not sleep with devices in their bedrooms, including TVs, computers, and smartphones.

4 stages of cognitive development according to Piaget
Image result for jean piaget
Constructivism & Jean Piaget

/